Locus coeruleus neurons: Cessation of activity during cataplexy

M.-F. Wu,* S. A. Gulyani, E. Yau, E. Mignot,§ B. Phan and J. M. Siegel†‡

†Neurobiology Research (151A3), VA Medical Center, North Hills, CA 91343, U.S.A., Department of Psychiatry and Brain Research Institute, University of California at Los Angeles, CA 91020, U.S.A. and §Stanford University, School of Medicine, Sleep Disorders Center, 701 Welch Road, Suite 2226, Palo Alto, CA 94304, U.S.A.

*To whom correspondence should be addressed

Neurobiology Research (151A3)

Sepulveda VA Medical Center

16111 Plummer St.

North Hills, CA 91343

U.S.A.

FAX: 818-895-9575

Phone: 818-891-7711 ext. 7828

E-mail: narco@ucla.edu

Acknowledgment: The authors thank Heidi Fahringer for her excellent histological assistance. This work is supported by the Medical Research Service of the Department of Veterans Affairs, USPHS NS14610 and NS23724

Abbreviations: 5M, motor trigeminal nucleus; 5m, tract of the mesencephalic trigeminal nucleus; AQ, aqueduct; AW, active waking; BC, brachium conjunctivum; CAT, cataplexy; CG, central gray; EEG, electroencephalogram; EMG, electromyogram; EOG, electrooculogram; Hipp, hippocampal EEG; IC, inferior colliculus; LC, locus coeruleus; non-REM, non-rapid-eye-movement; QW, quiet waking; REM, rapid-eye-movement; SC, locus subcoeruleus; TH, tyrosine hydroxylase.

Abstract

Cataplexy, a symptom of narcolepsy, is a loss of muscle tone usually triggered by sudden, emotionally significant stimuli. We now report that locus coeruleus neurons cease discharge throughout cataplexy periods in canine narcoleptics. Locus coeruleus discharge rates during cataplexy were as low as or lower than those seen during REM sleep. Prazosin, an a 1 antagonist, and physostigmine, a cholinesterase inhibitor, both of which precipitate cataplexy, decreased locus coeruleus discharge rate. Our results are consistent with the hypothesis that locus coeruleus activity contributes to the maintenance of muscle tone in waking, and that reduction in locus coeruleus discharge plays a role in the loss of muscle tone in cataplexy and REM sleep. Our results also show that the complete cessation of locus coeruleus activity is not sufficient to trigger REM sleep in narcoleptics.

Key words: locus coeruleus; narcolepsy; cataplexy; neuronal activity; sleep; canine

 

Narcolepsy, hypothesized to be a disorder of the rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep mechanism47,54 is characterized by a cluster of distinct symptoms, including hypnagogic hallucinations, sleepiness, sleep paralysis and cataplexy. Cataplexy is a complete loss of skeletal muscle tone that occurs when the narcoleptic is awake and alert. It may be triggered by athletic activity, laughter, anger or other emotions of sudden onset, or may occur without precipitating acts or emotions.2,22,23 The EEG is activated during cataplexy periods, as in waking and REM sleep.2,22,23,53 The only time a loss of muscle tone comparable to cataplexy occurs in individuals without narcolepsy, is during REM sleep. Cataplexy allows us to separate the neuronal systems linked to muscle atonia from those linked to other aspects of REM sleep, including its dream mentation and absence of awareness of the environment.

The locus coeruleus (LC) provides noradrenergic input to the forebrain, cerebellum, brainstem and spinal cord.43,48 Research in monkeys, cats and rats has revealed that LC neurons are tonically active during both quiet and active waking.5,18,26,29 They phasically increase discharge during arousal, in response to stressors1,3 and with sensory stimulation.5,18,46 It has been hypothesized that the tonic activity of LC neurons facilitates information processing throughout the brain,6,45,51 and that it is required for attention to significant environmental stimuli,5,6,7,14,19,45 learning4,12,25,33 and the activation of immediate early genes in several brain regions in waking.11 LC neurons reduce their activity during non-REM sleep and cease discharge for an extended period only in REM sleep. It has been thought that the cessation of activity in LC and other aminergic cells is a key part of the mechanism triggering this state.6,26,29,30,31,45 In the current study we have recorded LC neurons during normal sleep states and during cataplexy in narcoleptic dogs to determine if changes in locus coeruleus activities are linked to cataplexy.

Experimental Procedures

Surgery

LC cells were recorded from the dorsal pons of four narcoleptic Doberman Pinschers using microwire recording techniques as described previously.53 Under methoxyflurane or isoflurane anesthesia, microdrives containing 32 and 64 m m stainless steel microwires (California Fine Wire Co., Grover Beach, CA) aimed at the LC area were implanted, with the tip of the electrodes 1 mm above the target area. The frontal sinus was opened and stainless steel screw electrodes were placed over sensorimotor cortex for electroencephalogram (EEG) and into the posterior orbit for electrooculogram (EOG) recording. For the recording of hippocampal theta activity, tripolar twisted electrodes, made of three 256 m m formvar-insulated stainless steel wires (California Fine Wire Co., Grover Beach, CA), were placed in the dorsal hippocampus. Electromyogram (EMG) activity was recorded from the dorsal neck muscles with Teflon-coated multistranded stainless steel wires (Cooner Wire, Chatsworth, CA).

Unit and physiological recording in sleep and cataplexy

The dogs were free to move around the chamber (1.2 m x 1.2 m x 1.2 m in size) during the recordings. Electrodes were moved in 80 m m steps until a cell or cells with signal-to-ratio of at least 2 to 1 were isolated. The activity of each cell was then characterized throughout sleep-waking states and during cataplexy. Unit pulses from the window discriminator along with the digitized action potentials, EEG, EOG, EMG and hippocampal activity were recorded continuously on a PC using Spike2 software (Cambridge Electronic Design Ltd., Cambridge, UK). Microwire signals were filtered, with roll off of signals below 300 Hz and above 10 kHz, and digitized at a frequency of 25 kHz. Cortical and hippocampal EEG, and EOG signals were filtered between 1 and 100 Hz and sampled at a rate of 200 Hz. EMG was filtered between 30 Hz and 3 kHz and sampled at 1 kHz. Polygraph records of the unit pulses and all the physiological recordings were taken simultaneously.

Baseline sleep/wake discharge profile for each cell was established across at least 2 complete sleep/wake cycles. Cataplexy was then induced either by the introduction of soft food (Pedigree, Inc., Vernon, CA, U.S.A.) or by giving novel toys or play objects to the dog. A minimum of 5 cataplexies (mean of >10 cataplexies) were observed during the recording of each cell. In some cases, the anticholinersterase physostigmine salicylate (50-100 m g/kg, i.v.; Forest Pharmaceuticals, Inc., St. Louis, MO, U.S.A.) and/or the a 1 antagonist prazosin hydrochloride (0.5 mg/kg, p.o.; UDL Laboratories, Inc., Rockford, IL, U.S.A.) was administered to increase the frequency and the intensity of cataplexy. Cells were held for recording for as long as needed to complete the experimental protocol, from between 6.5 hours up to 7 days, with an average of over 4 and a half days. Physostigmine administered i.v. produces increased cataplexy within 5 minutes of administration, while oral prazosin administration has its maximal effect after one and a half hours. Narcoleptic dogs display partial cataplexies in which only the hind limbs collapse and complete cataplexies in which all four limbs collapse and the whole body contacts the floor. Partial cataplexies typically last less than 5 sec while complete cataplexies may last from 10 sec to a few minutes. Because of the short duration of the partial cataplexy episodes, only complete cataplexies were used for quantification of the change in unit discharge rate during this state. The effect of prazosin on cell activity was assessed between one and a half hours to 2 hours after drug administration during a quiet waking state. The effect of physostigmine was assessed during waking, 5-10 min after injection.

Cells were classified as noradrenergic based on the criteria reported for LC cells in the rat, cat and monkey.1,5,18 These criteria include: 1) slow (1-2 Hz) and regular firing in quiet waking, 2) greatly reduced firing in REM sleep, 3) suppression of firing by systemic administration of a 2 agonist clonidine, 4) long duration (> 2 msec) action potentials, and 5) histological localization to the pontine region containing tyrosine hydroxylase-positive cells.

Clonidine hydrochloride was given either intravenously (Sigma, St. Louis, MO, U.S.A.; 2 m g/kg, dissolved in saline) or orally in tablet form (UDL Laboratories, Inc.; 0.2 mg/kg). The effect of clonidine on unit discharge was assessed 10-30 min after the intravenous injection or 1 hr after oral administration during a quiet waking state. Spike duration was determined using the averaged waveform taken over 200 sweeps. To characterize the temporal relationship between unit and EMG activity, both unit and EMG activity were averaged around the onset and offset of cataplexy. The time of cataplexy onset and offset was determined from the digitized EMG. The onset of cataplexy was defined as the point at the beginning of a period in which the EMG became completely atonic for at least half a second. The offset of cataplexy was defined as the point where EMG activity resumed. Unit activity and EMG were averaged 20 sec before and after the onset/offset points. A paired t-test was used to compare baseline and post-drug firing rates with clonidine, physostigmine and prazosin administration. To prevent drug interactions or contamination of normal sleep cycle observations we allowed a 3-hr recovery from physostigmine and clonidine and a 12-hr recovery from prazosin before any further observations.

Histology

At the completion of recording, iron was deposited by passing a 15 m A, 20 sec anodal current through the electrodes from which cells had been recorded. Dogs were sacrificed by first deeply anesthetizing them with sodium pentobarbital (50 mg/kg, i.v.), then perfusing transcardiacly with buffered saline and paraformaldehyde.

Sixty-micron brain sections were cut and processed for tyrosine hydroxylase (TH). Sections were rinsed 3 times in 0.1M Trizma buffered saline followed by a 48-hour incubation in a 1/500 dilution of primary rabbit antiserum to TH (Eugene Tech International Inc, Ridgefield Park, New Jersey) at 4 ° C. The dilution was prepared with a solution of 1% normal goat serum and 0.25% Triton X-100 in 0.1M Trizma buffered saline. This was followed by a 2.5-hour incubation in a 1/200 dilution of biotinylated goat anti-rabbit IgG (Vector Labs, Burlingame, CA). The tissue was then incubated for 2 hours with the avidin-biotin complex diluted 1/100 (Vector). Each incubation was followed by three rinses. Dilutions and rinses were with 1% normal goat serum in 0.1M Trizma buffered saline. The sections were then treated for 6 minutes with a 0.05% solution of 3,3’ diaminobenzidine and 0.01% hydrogen peroxide, rinsed and mounted on gel coated slides. Sections were then rinsed in 5% potassium ferrocyanide for Prussian blue reaction to stain for iron, counter stained with neutral red, cleared in xylene and coverslipped with Permount mounting medium. The electrode tracks and the TH-positive cells were visualized and plotted on a Nikon microscope equipped with plotting software (DiLog Instruments and Systems, Florida). The plotted images were then superimposed onto a dog brain atlas.37

Results

Anatomical identification of recorded LC cells

A total of 339 cells were recorded from the dorsal pontine area. The majority of these cells increased discharge during waking movements and also during REM sleep, relative to quiet waking and non-REM sleep values. Thirteen of the cells met the criteria for noradrenergic cells. They were recorded from 3 of the 4 dogs used (2, 4, and 7 LC cells respectively for the 3 dogs). These cells were located in TH-positive cell regions (Fig. 1 and 2). TH-positive cells in the narcoleptic dogs were scattered along the medial portion of the superior cerebellar peduncle in the pons and in areas of LC proper and locus subcoeruleus (SC). Nine of the thirteen noradrenergic cells were recorded from the rostral portion of the LC and SC, while the other four were recorded from the caudal part of the LC (Fig. 2). All of these cells showed similar behavior across the sleep cycle and in cataplexy.

Physiological and behavioral characteristics of the LC cells

All of the cells meeting the criteria for noradrenergic cells had spike duration greater than 2 msec, with an average of 2.83± 0.15 msec (Fig. 3). They all discharged slowly and regularly in quiet waking (QW), phasically increased firing during active waking (AW), slowed down considerably in non-REM sleep and had minimal discharge during REM sleep (average firing rate in mean Hz± S.E.M: AW, 1.20± 0.08; QW, 0.91± .0.09; non-REM sleep 0.18± 0.03 and REM sleep 0.04± 0.01; Fig. 3). They ceased discharge before REM sleep onset and resumed activity at a low rate several seconds before the termination of REM sleep. In all cells, the return of discharge coincided with the cessation of hippocampal theta (Fig. 4).

All of these cells responded to stimulation of several sensory modalities in both waking and sleep states. An example of response to auditory stimulation is shown in Figure 5. Response latency to an 85 dB, 3.8 kHz tone pip was 40 to 60 msec. All the putative noradrenergic cells tested (7 of 13) reduced firing after clonidine administration. The inhibition occurred within minutes of intravenous injection and lasted for 30 to 60 minutes (Fig. 6). The discharge rate was reduced by an average of 78.1± 4.8% (from 1.23± 1.2 to 0.26± 0.06 Hz, p < 0.001) 10 minutes following clonidine injection (Fig. 6 insert).

Behavior of LC cells during cataplexy and sleep

Cataplexy was operationally defined as an abrupt loss of muscle tone in the alert waking state triggered during play or food intake. Cataplexy was accompanied by pronounced hippocampal theta activity similar to that seen in REM sleep. Eyes typically remained open and could follow moving objects. However, spontaneous eye movements were minimal. Apnea often occurred at cataplexy onset.52 Cataplexy normally terminated abruptly with the dog rising to its feet. In other cases cataplexy might be followed by eye closure and rapid eye movements signifying REM sleep. During cataplexy, all presumptive noradrenergic cells ceased discharge or reduced discharge to an extent comparable to or greater than that seen in REM sleep (Fig. 7). This relationship was highly consistent, occurring in every cell and in every cataplexy period. The cells reduced discharge 2- 4 sec before the loss of muscle tone (Fig. 7). The cells resumed firing at a low level more than 10 sec before the termination of cataplexy, with a marked increase in discharge 2-4 seconds prior to the increase of muscle tone that terminated cataplexy (Fig. 7). They showed the same resumption of firing prior to the end of REM sleep, cf. Fig. 4. Cell activity during partial cataplexy (e.g., collapse of the hindlimbs) (Fig. 8) was similar to that during complete cataplexy: reduction or cessation of discharge a few seconds before the onset of partial cataplexy, silence during the period of reduced tone, and resumption of discharge with or before the recovery of EMG activity. Figure 9 shows the discharge profile of individual cells during cataplexy and other sleep/waking states. Average firing rate during cataplexy was 0.018± 0.003 Hz, not significantly different from the rate in REM sleep (0.04± 0.01), but significantly different from that of AW (1.20± 0.08), QW (0.91± .0.09), and non-REM sleep (0.18± 0.03), all p<0.001 with paired t-test.

Drug effects on LC discharge

Both prazosin and physostigmine at the doses used greatly increased the propensity for cataplexy. We found that prazosin reduced the firing rate of LC cells in QW by 59.4± 15.1% (p <0.001, n = 6; Fig. 10) at the time of its peak effect on cataplexy. Physostigmine also reduced the firing rate of three LC cells tested (34.8± 3.2%, p < 0.001), 5-10 min after i.v. administration during the period when the dogs began to show cataplexy (Fig. 10).

Discussion

The offset of LC activity coincident with cataplexy provides a cellular mechanism for cataplexy initiation and cessation, and can explain many of the pharmacological effects on cataplexy. All REM-sleep-off cells located both rostrally and caudally in the locus coeruleus ceased discharge in all cataplexy episodes. The a 1 blocker prazosin dramatically increases cataplexy in both narcoleptic dogs and humans.2,22,23,42 We find that prazosin, at doses that increase cataplexy, reduced LC discharge. Therefore prazosin appears to have a dual effect of reducing norepinephrine release by discharge reduction, and reducing the postsynaptic effect of norepinephrine by blocking a 1 receptors. The mechanism by which prazosin reduces the discharge rate of LC cells is unclear. Prazosin has no substantial inhibitory effect on LC cells in vitro.27,28 The drugs most clinically useful for blocking cataplexy are a 2 antagonists and tricyclic antidepressants, both of which increase the availability of norepinephrine.42

Physostigmine in doses that consistently increase cataplexy also reduced the discharge of LC neurons. This effect is probably not the result of a direct cholinergic activation of muscarinic receptors on LC neurons, because both systemic injections of physostigmine or of an irreversible AChE inhibitor soman16,17 as well as iontophoretic applications of muscarinic agonists into LC8, 16,24,34 increase LC discharge. These studies were done either in anesthetized animals or in vitro. The opposite effect seen in the current study could result from anesthesia used in some of the prior studies, a species difference or the effects of narcolepsy. However, it is more likely that both prazosin and physostigmine reduce LC activity indirectly through multisynaptic mechanisms. Physostigmine may stimulate cholinergic mechanisms in the pons that are known to be capable of precipitating a loss of muscle tone.51 Thus, it appears that cholinergic and noradrenergic systems both contribute to the action of systemically administered physostigmine.

The current finding demonstrates that the complete cessation of discharge in LC neurons during cataplexy is not sufficient to induce REM sleep. However, in some cases, sustained cataplexy attacks eventually terminate in REM sleep in dogs, as they do in humans. This indicates that the cessation of LC discharge, when combined with changes in the activity of cholinergic or other brainstem neuronal populations,53,54 can lead to REM sleep.

The major behavioral characteristic common to the states of cataplexy and REM sleep is muscle atonia. Lifting cats by the scruff of the neck, which causes a muscle atonia response and associated autonomic changes, is the only behavioral manipulation associated with a great reduction in LC discharge in waking.49 These findings suggest a strong correlation between LC activity and muscle tone. One third of LC neurons and the majority of non-LC noradrenergic cells have descending projections to spinal and brainstem motoneurons.32,39,43,48,50 A number of studies have demonstrated that norepinephrine released by the descending projections of LC neurons potently facilitates activity in spinal motoneurons, presumably acting through a 1 adrenoceptors in the ventral horn.13,20,21,44,57 Unilateral or bilateral lesions of the locus coeruleus, or chemical inactivation by clonidine microinjection, produce a reduction of ipsilateral or bilateral muscle tone.13 Thus cessation of LC discharge in REM sleep and cataplexy would disfacilitate motor activity. LC neurons also have strong projections to pontine and medullary regions involved in the circuit responsible for the hyperpolarization of motoneurons,9 a circuit that is activated both in cataplexy and in REM sleep.54 We have previously shown that cataplexy is accompanied by increased discharge in a population of cells in the nucleus magnocellularis of the medulla,53 the medullary inhibitory area. Stimulation of this area produces suppression of muscle tone.36 These results suggest that REM sleep atonia and the atonia of cataplexy result from aminergic disfacilitation,35 coordinated with active inhibition of motoneurons.10

Locus coeruleus cells have major ascending projections.31 These projections have been hypothesized to have a role in attention.7 The ascending projections of locus coeruleus cells may help facilitate forebrain processing of sensory inputs generated by motor activity.

Blood pressure elevation, induced by intravenous infusion of norepinephrine or angiotensin II, causes an immediate reduction in LC cell discharge15 while nitroprusside-induced hypotension increases the discharge of LC neurons.58 Since cataplexy is normally triggered by emotional events, one may speculate that changes in blood pressure are linked to the cessation of LC activity during cataplexy. However, no consistent blood pressure change occurs during the onset of canine cataplexy.52 Furthermore, the transient increase of heart rate that we saw prior to cataplexy onset returns to baseline values52 and therefore cannot account for the prolonged inactivation of LC cells throughout cataplexy period.

The loss of consciousness that accompanies slow wave sleep can be seen as resulting from the occlusion of sensory processing by the synchronized rhythmicity of thalamo-cortical circuits.55,56 However, both REM sleep and waking are characterized by a low voltage EEG. This pattern, generated by thalamo-cortical interactions, is under the control of projections from brainstem cholinergic and aminergic cells. Despite the similarity of cortical EEG in waking and REM sleep, the organism is unconscious and experiences dream mentation only during REM sleep. Humans report unimpaired consciousness and memory for events transpiring during cataplexy.22 Narcoleptic dogs show their awareness of the environment during cataplexy by visually tracking objects of interest.41 The activity of aminergic cells groups in waking, and their inactivity during REM sleep was a possible explanation for the difference in the nature of consciousness in these two states. However, the current results demonstrate that awareness of the environment does not require the activity of LC cells in narcoleptic dogs. These results either reflect the independence of LC cell activity from the maintenance of awareness or represent a CNS adaptation in narcoleptics to the periodic loss of activity in locus coeruleus cells. It remains to be seen if serotonergic and histaminergic cells, the other two groups known to be tonically active in waking and silent in REM sleep,38,40,59 are silent during cataplectic attacks.

Conclusion

The present results show a strong correlation between the cessation of LC activity and cataplexy. Drugs that enhance cataplexy reduced spontaneous LC activity during the time that the propensity for cataplexy was enhanced. These results suggest that cessation of LC activity and consequent disfacilitation of motoneurons is critical for the initiation of cataplexy. Cessation of discharge in the LC may act synergistically with activation of the medullary atonia system to produce motor inhibition in REM sleep and cataplexy.

REFERENCES

1. Abercrombie, E. D. and Jacobs, B. L. (1987) Single-Unit response of noradrenergic neurons in the locus coeruleus of freely moving cats. I. Acutely presented stressful and nonstressful stimuli. J. Neurosci. 7, 2837-2843.

2. Aldrich, M. S. (1990) Narcolepsy. New England J. Med. 323, 389-394.

3. Amaral, D. G. and Sinnamon, H. M. (1977) The locus coeruleus: neurobiology of a central noradrenergic nucleus. Prog. Neurobiol. 9, 147-196.

4. Anlezark, G., Crow, T. and Greenway, A. (1973) Impaired learning and decreased cortical norepinephrine after bilateral locus coeruleus lesions. Science 181, 682-684.

5. Aston-Jones, G. and Bloom, F. E. (1981) Activity of norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats anticipates fluctuations in the sleep-waking cycle. J. Neurosci. 1, 876-886.

6. Aston-Jones, G, Foote, S. L. and Bloom, F. E. (1984) Norepinephrine. In Norepinephrine (Ed. Ziegler, M. and Lake, R.), pp. 92-116. Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore.

7. Aston-Jones, G., Rajkowski, J., Kubiak, P. and Alexinsky, T. (1994) Locus coeruleus neurons in monkey are selectively activated by attended cues in a vigilance task. J. Neurosci. 14, 4467-4480.

8. Bird, S. and Kuhar, M. J. (1977) Iontophoretic application of opiates to the locus coeruleus. Brain Res. 122, 523-533.

9. Bowden, D., German, D. and Poynter, W. (1978) An autoradiographic, semistereotaxic mapping of major projections from locus coeruleus and adjacent nuclei in Macaca mulatta. Brain Res. 145, 257-276.

10. Chase, M. H. and Morales, F. R. (1990) The atonia and myoclonia of active (REM) sleep. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 41, 557-584.

11. Cirelli, C., Pompeiano, M. and Tononi, G. (1996) Neuronal gene expression in the waking state: a role for the locus coeruleus. Science 274, 1211-1217.

12. Cornwell-Jones, C. A., Decker, M. W., Chang, J. W., Cole, B., Goltz, K. M., Tran, T. and Mcgaugh, J. L. (1989) Neonatal 6-Hydroxydopa, but not DSP-4, elevates brainstem monoamines and impairs inhibitory avoidance learning in developing rats. Brain Res. 493, 258-268.

13. D'Ascanio P, Pompeiano O and Stampacchia G (1988) Noradrenergic and cholinergic mechanisms responsible for the gain regulation of vestibulospinal reflexes. Prog. Brain Res. 76, 361-374.

14. Devauges, V. and Sara, S. J. (1990) Activation of the noradrenergic system facilitates an attentional shift in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 39, 19-28.

15. Elam, M., Yao, T., Svensson, T. H. and Thoren, P. (1984) Regulation of locus coeruleus neurons and splanchnic, sympathetic nerves by cardiovascular afferents. Brain Res. 290, 281-287.

16. Engberg, G. and Svensson, T. H. (1980) Pharmacological analysis of a cholinergic receptor mediated regulation of brain norepinephrine neurons. J. Neural Trans. 49, 137-150.

17. Ennis, M. and Shipley, M. T. (1992) Tonic activation of locus-coeruleus neurons by systemic or intracoerulear microinjection of an irreversible acetylcholinesterase inhibitor - increased discharge rate and induction of c-fos. Exp. Neurol. 118, 164-177.

18. Foote, S. L., Astone-Jones, G. and Bloom, F. E. (1980) Impulse activity of locus coeruleus neurons in awake rats and monkeys is a function of sensory stimulation and arousal. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 77, 3033-3037.

19. Foote, S. L., Bloom, F. E. and Aston-Jones, G. (1983) Nucleus locus coeruleus: new evidence of anatomical and physiological specificity. Physiol. Rev. 63, 844-914.

20. Fung, S. J., Manzoni, D., Chan, J. Y. H., Pompeiano, O. and Barnes, C. D. (1991) Locus coeruleus control of spinal motor output. Prog. Brain Res. 88, 395-409.

21. Fung S. J., Pompeiano O and Barnes C. D. (1988) Coerulospinal influence on recurrent inhibition of spinal motonuclei innervating antagonistic hindleg muscles of the cat. Pflugers Archive 412, 346-353.

22. Guilleminault, C. (1976) Narcolepsy. In Narcolepsy (ed. Guilleminault, C., Dement, W. C. and Passouant, P.), pp. 125-143. Spectrum, New York.

23. Guilleminault, C. (1994) Narcolepsy syndrome. In Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine (Ed. Kryger, M. H., Roth, T. and Dement, W. C.), pp. 145-162. W. B. Saunders Company, Philadelphia.

24. Guyenet, P. G. and Aghajanian, G. K. (1979) ACh, substance P and met-enkephalin in the locus coeruleus: pharmacological evidence for independent sites of action. Europ. J. Pharmacol. 53, 319-328.

25. Harley C. W. (1987) A role for norepinephrine in arousal, emotion and learning?: limbic modulation by norepinephrine and the Kety hypothesis. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. & Biol. Psychiat. 11, 419-458.

26. Hobson, J. A., McCarley, R. W. and Wyzinski, P. W. (1975) Sleep cycle oscillation: reciprocal discharge by two brainstem neuronal groups. Science 189, 55-58.

27. Illes, P. and Norenberg, W. (1990) Blockade of a2-adrenoceptors increases opioid a-receptor-mediated inhibition of the firing rate of rat locus coeruleus neurons. Naunyn-Schmiedeberg's Arch. Pharmacol. 342, 490-496.

28. Ivanov, A. and Aston-Jones, G. (1995) Extranuclear dendrites of locus coeruleus neurons: activation by glutamate and modulation of activity by alpha adrenoceptors. J. Neurophysiol. 74, 2427-2436.

29. Jacobs B. L. (1986) Single Unit activity of locus coeruleus neurons in behaving animals. Prog. Neurobiol. 27, 183-194.

30. Jacobs, B. L., Heym, J. and Trulson, M. E. (1981) Behavioral and physiological correlates of brain serotoninergic unit activity. J. Physiol. 77, 431-436.

31. Jones, B. E. (1994) Basic mechanisms of sleep-wake states. In Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine (ed. Kryger, M. H., Roth, T. and Dement, W. C.), pp. 145-162. W. B. Saunders Company, Philadelphia.

32. Jones, B. E. (1991) Noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons: their distant connections and their relationship to neighboring (including cholinergic and GABAergic) neurons of the central gray and reticular formation. Prog. Brain Res. 88, 15-30.

33. Kasamatsu, T. and Pettigrew, J. (1976) Depletion of brain catecholamines: failure of ocular dominance shift after monocular occlusion in kittens. Science 194, 206-209.

34. Koyama, Y. and Kayama, Y. (1993) Mutual interactions among cholinergic, noradrenergic and serotonergic neurons studied by ionophoresis of these transmitters in rat brainstem nuclei. Neurosci. 55, 1117-1126.

35. Kubin, L., Reignier, C., Tojima, H., Taguchi, O., Pack, A. I. and Davis, R. O. (1994) Changes in serotonin level in the hypoglossal nucleus region during carbachol-induced atonia. Brain Res. 645, 291-302.

36. Lai, Y. Y. and Siegel, J. M. (1988) Medullary regions mediating atonia. J. Neurosci. 8, 4790-4796.

37. Lim, R. K. S., Liu, C.-N. and Moffitt, R. L. (1960) A Stereotaxic Atlas of the Dog's Brain. Charles C. Thomas, Publisher, Speingfield, Illinois.

38. Lin, J. S., Sakai, K. and Jouvet, M. (1986) Role of hypothalamic histaminergic systems in the regulation of vigilance states in cats. C. R. Acad. Sci. Paris, 303, 469-474.

39. Lyons, W. E., Fritschy, J. M. and Grzanna, R. (1989) The noradrenergic neurotoxin DSP-4 eliminates the coerulospinal projection but spares projections of the A5 and A7 groups to the ventral horn of the rat spinal cord. J. Neurosci. 9, 1481-1489.

40. McGinty, D. J. and Harper, R. M. (1976) Dorsal raphe neurons: depression of firing rates during sleep in cats. Brain Res. 101, 569-575.

41. Mitler, M. M., Soave, O. and Dement, W. C. (1976) Narcolepsy in seven dogs. J.A.V.M.A. 168, 1036-1038.

42. Nishino, S. and Mignot, E. (1997) Pharmacological aspects of human and canine narcolepsy. Prog. Neurobiol. 52, 27-78.

43. Nygren, L. G. and Olson, L. (1977) A new major projection from locus coeruleus: the main source of noradrenergic nerve terminals in the ventral and dorsal columns of the spinal cord. Brain Res. 132, 85-93.

44. Palmeri, A. and Wiesendanger, M. (1990) Concomitant depression of locus coeruleus neurons and of flexor reflexes by an alpha2-adrenergic agonist in rats: a possible mechanism for an alpha2-mediated muscle relaxation. Neurosci. 34, 177-187.

45. Pineda, J. A., Foote, S. L. and Neville, H. J. (1989) Effects of locus coeruleus lesions on auditory, long-latency, event-related potentials in monkey. J. Neurosci. 9, 81-93.

46. Rasmussen, K., Morilak, D. A. and Jacobs, B. L. (1986) Single unit response of locus coeruleus neurons in the freely moving cat. I. During naturalistic behaviors and in response to simple and complex stimuli. Brain Res. 371, 324-334.

47. Rechtschaffen, A., Wolpert, E. A., Dement, W. C., Mitchel, S. A. and Fisher, C. (1963) Nocturnal sleep of narcoleptics. Electroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol. 15, 599-609.

48. Reddy, V. K., Fung, S. J., Zhuo, H. and Barnes, C. D. (1989) Spinally projecting noradrenergic neurons of the dorsolateral pontine tegmentum: a combined immunocytochemical and retrograde labeling study. Brain Res. 491, 144-149.

49. Reiner, P. B. (1986) Correlational analysis of central noradrenergic neuronal activity and sympathetic tone in behaving cats. Brain Res. 378, 86-96.

50. Satoh, K., Tohyama, M., Yamamoto, K. S. T. and Shimizu, N. (1977) Noradrenaline innervation of the spinal cord studied by the horseradish peroxidase method combined with monoamine oxidase staining. Exp. Brain Res. 30, 175-186.

51. Siegel, J. M. (1994) Brain mechanisms generating REM sleep. In Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine (Ed. Kryger, M. H., Roth, T. and Dement, W. C.), pp. 125-144. W. B. Saunders Company, Philadelphia.

52. Siegel, J. M., Fahringer, H., Tomazewski, K. S., Kaitin, K., Kilduff, T. and Dement, W. C. (1986) Heart Rate and blood pressure changes associated with cataplexy in canine narcolepsy. Sleep 9, 216-221.

53. Siegel, J. M., Nienhuis, R., Fahringer, H. M., Chiu, C., Dement, W. C., Mignot, E. and Lufkin, R. (1992) Activity of medial mesopontine units during cataplexy and sleep-waking states in the narcoleptic dog. J. Neurosci. 12, 1640-1646.

54. Siegel, J. M., Nienhuis, R., Fahringer, H. M., Paul, R., Shiromani, P., Dement, W. C., Mignot, E. and Chiu, C. (1991) Neuronal activity in narcolepsy: identification of cataplexy-related cells in the medial medulla. Science 252, 1315-1318.

55. Steriade, M. (1996) Arousal: revisiting the reticular activating system [comment]. Science 272, 225-226.

56. Steriade, M., McCormick, D. A. and Sejnowski, T. J. (1993) Thalamocortical oscillations in the sleeping and aroused brain. Science 262, 679-685.

57. Tanabe, M., Ono, H. and Fukuda, H. (1990) Spinal alpha 1- and alpha 2-adrenoceptors mediate facilitation and inhibition of spinal motor transmission, respectively. Japanese J. Pharmacol. 54, 69-77.

58. Valentino, R. J., Page, M. E. and Curtis, A. L. (1991) Activation of noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons by hemodynamic stress is due to local release of corticotrophin-releasing factor. Brain Res. 555, 25-34.

59. Vanni-Mercier, G., Sakai, K. and Jouvet, M. (1984) Waking-state specific neurons in the caudal hypothalamus of the cat. C.R. Acad. Sci. Paris, 298, 195-200.

 

 

Fig. 1. Coronal section at R3 level showing lesion marks made at the electrode tips (arrows) in a region of TH-positive cells (stained brown). Calibration: 0.5 mm.

Fig. 2. Coronal plots at R3 and R7 levels showing location of recorded REM-cataplexy-off cells on the left and tyrosine hydroxylase stained cells on the right. REM-cataplexy-off cells were all in regions with TH-positive cells.

 

Fig. 3. Discharge patterns of a noradrenergic REM-cataplexy-off cell during quite waking (QW), non-REM sleep (nonREM) and REM sleep (REM). The insert depicts the spike waveform of the same cell. It has a spike width of 2.8 msec. Unit, computer triggered spike display.

 

 

 

Fig. 4. Discharge pattern of a noradrenergic REM-cataplexy-off cell during the transition from REM sleep to waking. Note that discharge of the cell resumed about 16.6 sec before the end of REM sleep, as indicated by the recovery of EMG activity. The cell began firing at a low rate coincident with the end of REM sleep hippocampal theta activity (arrow).

 

 

 

Fig. 5. Histogram showing the responses of an LC cell to 20 msec, 80 dB, 3.8 kHz tone pips. A total of 20 trials are delivered. LC cells respond to auditory stimuli with a latency of 40-60 msec.

 

Fig. 6. Effects of clonidine (2 m g/kg, i.v.) on the spontaneous discharge of an LC cell. Clonidine produced a 86% reduction in the discharge of this cell 10 min after injection as compared to its immediate pre-drug level. Clonidine reduced discharge for about 45 min. The reduction of discharge by clonidine, a consequence of a 2 receptor activation, is characteristic of noradrenergic cells. Insert: Average discharge rate (Mean± S.E.M.) before and after clonidine administration in the seven cells tested. Baseline discharge rates were taken during the 10 min period before drug administration in QW. The effect of clonidine was assessed 10-30 min (i.v.; n=6) or 1 hr (oral; n=1) after drug administration during QW. Asterisk (*) denote significant difference (p <0.001, paired t-test) in firing rate between baseline and post-drug condition.

 

Fig. 7. Locus coeruleus discharge during cataplexy. We were able to simultaneously record four REM-cataplexy-off cells throughout sleep/waking states and during episodes of cataplexy. Top: Unit activity and polygraph traces during cataplexy onset (A) and offset (B). Note the cessation of discharge at cataplexy onset (A) and the resumption of discharge shortly before the termination of cataplexy (B). Bottom: Averaged spike histogram and EMG before and after cataplexy onset (C) and offset (D). Twenty cataplexy episodes (mean duration 23.7± 3.7 sec) were averaged across the four cells displayed above. The bin size of the histogram is 400 msec.

 

 

Fig 8. The activity of two simultaneously recorded REM-cataplexy-off cells during partial cataplexies (gray bars). These cells ceased activity before and during partial cataplexies and resumed firing at or before the recovery of EMG activity, similar to their behavior during complete cataplexies. The EMG signal was full-wave rectified and is displayed as a moving-time average with 500 msec time constant.

 

Fig. 9. Discharge rate profile of REM-cataplexy-off cells recorded across all sleep/waking states and during cataplexy. Rate is minimal in the atonic states of cataplexy and REM sleep, higher in non-REM sleep and highest in waking.