|
The Journal of Neuroscience, January 1, 1999, 19(1):248-257
1 Veterans Administration Medical Center Sepulveda and Department of Psychiatry and Brain Research Institute, University of California Los Angeles School of Medicine, Neurobiology Research 151A3, Sepulveda, California 91343, 2 Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Sleep Research Center, Richard Lucas/Lab Surge Building, Palo Alto, California 94304, 3 NeuroScience Associates, Knoxville, Tennessee 37922, and 4 Department of Pathology, Harbor University of California Los Angeles Medical Center, Torrance, California 90509
![]() |
ABSTRACT |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Narcolepsy is a lifelong illness characterized by persistent sleepiness, hypnagogic hallucinations, and episodes of motor paralysis called cataplexy. We have tested the hypothesis that a transient neurodegenerative process is linked to symptom onset. Using the amino-cupric silver stain on brain sections from canine narcoleptics, we found elevated levels of axonal degeneration in the amygdala, basal forebrain (including the nucleus of the diagonal band, substantia innominata, and preoptic region), entopeduncular nucleus, and medial septal region. Reactive neuronal somata, an indicator of neuronal pathology, were found in the ventral amygdala. Axonal degeneration was maximal at 2-4 months of age. The number of reactive cells was maximal at 1 month of age. These degenerative changes precede or coincide with symptom onset. The forebrain degeneration that we have observed can explain the major symptoms of narcolepsy.
Key words: narcolepsy; REM sleep; amygdala; basal forebrain; canine; amino-cupric silver; degeneration; cataplexy
![]() |
INTRODUCTION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Narcolepsy, which occurs at a rate of 0.2-1.6 per thousand (Aldrich, 1990;
Hublin et al., 1994
),
was first recognized 118 years ago by Gélineau (Passouant,
1976
).
Its symptoms include excessive daytime sleepiness, hypnagogic hallucinations
(dream-like mentation in waking), REM sleep at sleep onset, cataplexy
(a loss of muscle tone in waking, usually triggered by sudden, strong
emotions), and sleep paralysis (an inability to move at sleep onset
or awakening) (Guilleminault, 1994
).
Narcolepsy has been reported in horses, cattle, and dogs (Mitler
et al., 1976
;
Strain et al., 1984
).
Canine narcoleptics have been intensively studied. Like human narcoleptics,
they are excessively sleepy and have cataplexy. Symptoms in canine
and human narcoleptics display a similar response to pharmacological
agents (Guilleminault, 1994
;
Nishino and Mignot, 1997
).
The cause of narcolepsy is unknown.
Narcolepsy is not a progressive disease, in that once symptoms have become
fully established, in both human and canine narcoleptics, they do
not become worse (or markedly better) with age. This suggests that
narcolepsy may be caused by a transient degenerative process. Examinations
of postmortem tissue in human narcoleptics have not produced consistent
evidence for degenerative changes. However, symptom onset is typically
50 or more years before autopsy, a sufficient interval for the
removal of any debris resulting from degeneration at the time of
disease onset. The age of onset of canine narcolepsy is between 1 and
4 months. In the current study, we have used the amino-cupric
stain (de Olmos et al., 1994),
an extremely sensitive indicator of degenerating neurons and axons
(Switzer, 1991
;
Fix et al., 1996
),
to test the hypothesis that narcolepsy onset is linked to neuronal
degeneration.
![]() |
MATERIALS AND METHODS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Eighteen Doberman pinscher dogs, nine narcoleptic and nine age- and breed-matched controls (four male narcoleptics and seven male controls), from six narcoleptic and five normal litters ranging from 1 to 8 months of age were used (Table 1). Control and narcoleptic dogs were reared under similar conditions and never given any pharmacological agents before killing. They were anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (50 mg/kg) and perfused with a rinsing solution of 0.8% sucrose, 0.4% glucose, and 0.8% NaCl in 0.067 M cacodylate buffer, pH 7.3. Fixation was with 4% formaldehyde in 0.067 M cacodylate buffer containing 4% sucrose. After the brains were allowed to harden in situ for 24 hr, they were removed from the skull and placed in fixative for 7 d.
|
Amino-cupric protocol
Embedding and sectioning
Brains were treated with 20% glycerol and American Optical dimethylsulfoxide to prevent freeze artifacts. Two half brains (a narcoleptic and control) were embedded side by side, with their medial surfaces aligned using the anterior commissure as a landmark. The block of embedded brains was allowed to cure and then rapidly frozen by immersion in isopentane chilled toStaining
Selection of sections for staining. A serial set of every sixth section (a 240 µm interval) was selected for staining with the amino-cupric-silver stain of de Olmos (1994)
|
![]() |
RESULTS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Axonal degeneration was elevated in the amygdala, septal nucleus, diagonal band of Broca, and adjacent basal forebrain regions of narcoleptics (Fig. 1a-f). Narcoleptics had significantly higher levels of degeneration across the entire age spectrum than age-matched controls (F = 15.6, 8, and 124 df; p < .0001). There was no significant difference between amounts of degeneration in male and female dogs within either group, or in the ratio of numbers of degenerating axons or reactive cells in male-female or same sex pairs of narcoleptics and controls. At all ages, and in every pair of narcoleptic-control brains examined, the narcoleptic half had higher levels of degeneration than the control (Wilcoxon test, t = 8.4; p < .01).
Certain structures contained degenerating axons at 1 month of age (Fig. 2) but not at later ages. At 1 month of age, intense labeling was seen in the medial septal nucleus, diagonal band, fornix, magnocellular preoptic region, substantia innominata, entopeduncular nucleus, and pyriform cortex. Within the amygdala, the basalis magnocellularis, central, lateral, and anterior nuclei were most heavily labeled. Figure 3 shows the distribution of degenerating axons at 6 months of age. At 2-6 months of age, degeneration was seen in medial septal nucleus, diagonal band, amygdala, and pyriform cortex, but not in the fornix, substantia innominata, magnocellular preoptic, or entopeduncular nucleus.
|
|
Axonal degeneration was maximal at 1-3 months of age, with the level of degeneration
greatly decreased by 8 months of age (Fig. 4).
Increased levels of degeneration were present in the septal nucleus,
but not in the amygdala, between 6 and 8 months of age
(Fig. 4). Despite the role of the brainstem in REM sleep
generation (Siegel, 1994)
and the involvement of brainstem efferent mechanisms in cataplexy
(Siegel et al., 1991
),
we saw no evidence for elevated levels of degeneration in the brainstem
of the narcoleptic dogs at any age.
|
Stained neuronal somata were seen at increased levels in the amygdala of narcoleptics
from 1 to 4 months of age (Fig. 1g-i).
Cell death preceded by reactivity to cupric silver and followed by
rapid lysis of the soma may be the primary event, with axonal degeneration
and relatively long-lasting axonal debris fields as a consequence
(Switzer, 1991;
de Olmos, 1994
;
Fix et al., 1996
).
Conversely, cells may become reactive to the cupric silver stain
during the chromatolytic reaction resulting from axonal loss (Switzer,
1991
; de Olmos
et al., 1994
). The
highest levels of reactive cells were at 1 month of age (Figs.
5, 6), with a majority of the labeled
cells in the ventral amygdala and pyriform cortex. We stained adjacent
sections of tissue from 2-month-old narcoleptics with the TUNEL stain
(Oncor), an indicator of apoptosis, but did not see any labeling.
|
|
![]() |
DISCUSSION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
We found that degeneration was present in canine narcoleptics at or shortly before the time of symptom onset. Degeneration was present in areas that have been implicated in response to startle and in sleep control.
No consistent evidence for neuronal or axonal degeneration has been reported
in any brain region in human narcoleptics. However, a few cases of
"symptomatic" narcolepsy linked to tumors or other lesions have been
seen. Most patients with symptomatic narcolepsy have been reported
to have diencephalic-basal forebrain-septal nucleus damage, whereas
few had any brainstem pathology (Stahl et al., 1980;
Erlich and Itabashi, 1986
;
Aldrich and Naylor, 1989
;
Servan et al., 1995
).
A recent report of brainstem lesions in narcolepsy (Plazzi et al.,
1996
) has been disputed
(Bassetti et al., 1997
;
Frey and Heiserman, 1997
).
Gross anatomical lesions caused by neoplasms or strokes are absent
in the vast majority of human cases (Aldrich, 1990
).
The time course of the degenerative process in the narcoleptic dogs paralleled
the lower levels of axonal and neuronal degeneration in each brain
region seen at the same stage of development in the control dogs,
with both peaking at 1-3 months. In the dogs, symptom onset occurs
relatively early in life, at 1-4 months of age (Mignot et al., 1993),
consistent with the observed neuronal degeneration. Human narcolepsy
has been seen in children as young as 3 years (Yoss and Daly,
1960
; Billiard,
1985
; Kotagal et
al., 1990
;
Challamel et al., 1994
)
but typically starts in the second or third decade (Aldrich, 1990
).
Our time course data suggest two possible scenarios for a comparable
degenerative process in humans. The first is that degeneration could
occur at the age of disease onset with no previous abnormality. The
second possibility is that degeneration could occur early in development
in narcoleptic humans, with some subsequent degenerative or hormonal
process triggering the disease at a later age.
The latter time course would resemble that of the degenerative process thought
to occur in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia, like narcolepsy, is correlated
with degeneration that includes portions of the amygdala and other
frontotemporal regions (Bogerts, 1993;
Marsh et al., 1994
;
Nasrallah et al., 1994
).
The best evidence is that the damage in schizophrenics occurs prenatally
or early in development (Bogerts, 1993
),
as we find in canine narcolepsy. Like narcolepsy, symptoms of schizophrenia
are usually not present in early childhood. Symptom onset in schizophrenics
is typically in the second or third decade and, as in narcolepsy,
damage does not appear to be progressive (Marsh et al., 1994
).
Most narcoleptics have hypnagogic hallucinations, a symptom with
some resemblance to the hallucinatory mentation of certain schizophrenics.
Several cases of schizophrenia coexisting with or misdiagnosed as
narcolepsy have been reported (Cadieux et al., 1985
;
Douglass et al., 1991
).
The amygdala is one of the forebrain areas most strongly activated in REM sleep
(Maquet et al., 1996;
Nofsinger et al., 1997
).
Amygdala stimulation in normal cats potently increases REM sleep
duration (Calvo et al., 1996
).
The amygdala is also known to be involved in the elaboration of emotional
responses and has a powerful role in the modulation of startle (Campeau
and Davis, 1995
).
There are major projections from the amygdala to the dorsolateral
pontine cholinergic and noradrenergic cell regions involved in the
generation of REM sleep phenomena (Wallace et al., 1992
).
We hypothesize that the loss of neurons within the amygdala, basal
forebrain, and septal region disinhibits amygdala cells projecting
to the brainstem. These disinhibited cells are activated during sudden,
strong emotions. This triggers the brainstem motor inhibitory system
and inactivates the locus coeruleus (Wu et al., 1998
),
resulting in cataplexy. It has been shown that activation of the
amygdala produces EKG acceleration and apnea (Frysinger et al., 1984
),
changes that also occur at the onset of cataplexy (Siegel et al.,
1989
).
The entopeduncular nucleus, like the amygdala, is important in the elaboration
of emotional responses and has a particularly important role in the
recognition of rewarding events (Hammer et al., 1993;
Breiter et al., 1997
).
Pleasurable stimuli, including food ingestion, the most reliable
trigger of canine cataplexy, activate the entopeduncular nucleus
(Lidsky, 1975
; Schneider,
1987
). As
in the amygdala, degenerative changes that alter circuitry or disinhibit
cells could be responsible for an abnormal output from this region
to the amygdala and brainstem regions (Schneider, 1987
).
The septal nucleus is known to have important arousal and startle-related functions.
Electrolytic lesions of the septum produce a dramatic exaggeration
of the startle response (McCleary, 1961).
Cholinergic and GABAergic neurons localized to the medial septal
region project to limbic structures and produce the theta rhythm
in the hippocampus (Vertes and Kocsis, 1997
),
a rhythm that is prominent in both REM sleep and cataplexy (Wu et
al., 1998
).
The amygdala, diagonal band of Broca, and magnocellular preoptic region are
the major components of the basal forebrain hypnogenic region. Sleep-active
neurons, hypothesized to be involved in sleep induction, are localized
to this area (Szymusiak and McGinty, 1986b).
Stimulation of the ventral amygdala produces EEG synchrony (Kreindler
and Steriade, 1964
).
Stimulation of the preoptic region also induces sleep (Sterman and
Clemente, 1962
).
Lesions of this area produce the most profound insomnia seen after
any brain lesion (Szymusiak and McGinty, 1986a
).
Narcoleptic canines have elevated levels of dopaminergic and noradrenergic
receptors in the amygdala, brainstem, and basal forebrain (Mefford
et al., 1983
; Kilduff
et al., 1986
).
Similar changes are present in human narcoleptics (Aldrich et al.,
1992
, 1993
,
1994
). Cholinergic
stimulation of the basal forebrain triggers cataplexy in narcoleptic,
but not in control canines (Nishino et al., 1995
).
Disinhibition of the basal forebrain region by loss of local interneurons
could produce the major non-REM sleep-related symptoms of narcolepsy,
disruption of nighttime sleep and excessive daytime sleepiness (Aldrich,
1991
), as well
as the reported changes in receptor levels. Thus, the degeneration
we see in amygdala, basal forebrain, and septum are consistent with
the EEG, motor, and sleepiness symptoms of narcolepsy.
Human narcolepsy is correlated with the presence of the human leukocyte antigen
(HLA) DQB1*0602 genotype (Matsuki et al., 1992).
The association of narcolepsy with the major histocompatibility complex
marker, HLA-DR2 and DQB1*0602, is one of the highest disease-HLA linkages
known (Behar et al., 1995
).
Most HLA-linked disorders have been shown to be autoimmune in nature
(Sinha et al., 1990
).
Canine narcolepsy is linked to the presence of a marker for an Ig
switch-like sequence (Mignot et al., 1991
)
and enhanced microglial expression (Tafti et al., 1996
)
at 1-3 months of age. These findings all suggest that immune processes,
perhaps related to axonal pruning or cell necrosis, may be linked
to narcolepsy onset. Consistent evidence for immune abnormalities
in human and canine narcolepsy have not been found, indicating that
narcolepsy probably does not involve long-term generalized autoimmune
activation (Fredrikson et al., 1990
;
Mignot et al., 1995
).
However, autoimmune processes linked to a localized, time-limited
degenerative process, preceding symptom onset would be missed by
the techniques used to look for autoimmune processes in previous
studies. The degenerative changes we have observed could form the
link between autoimmune activation and the abnormalities of motor
and sleep function that characterize narcolepsy.
![]() |
FOOTNOTES |
---|
Received June 15, 1998; revised Oct. 2, 1998; accepted Oct. 12, 1998.
This work was supported by the Medical Research Service of the Veterans Administration, United States Public Health Service Grants NS14610 and NS23724.
Correspondence should be addressed to Jerome Siegel, Department of Psychiatry University of California Los Angeles, Neurobiology Research 151A3, Veterans Administration Medical Center, 16111 Plummer Street, North Hills, CA 91343.
![]() |
REFERENCES |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
|